Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-13T03:09:21.197Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Who Is Responsible for the Emergency Aid? Cash Transfer and Presidential Approval During the COVID-19 Pandemic in Brazil

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2023

Frederico Batista Pereira
Affiliation:
Frederico Batista Pereira is an assistant professor at the University of North Carolina at Charlotte, Charlotte, NC, USA. fbatist1@uncc.edu.
Guilherme Russo
Affiliation:
Guilherme Russo is head of research at Quaest Consultoria e Pesquisa and is a lecturer at the São Paulo School of Economics, Getúlio Vargas Foundation (FGV), São Paulo, Brazil. guilherme.russo@quaest.com.br.
Felipe Nunes
Affiliation:
Felipe Nunes is CEO of Quaest Consultoria e Pesquisa and is an associate professor at the Federal University of Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil. felipnunes@gmail.com.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Studies show that cash transfer programs increase incumbent approval through their financial impact and clear association with the executive. But does this effect hold when it is the legislature rather than the incumbent proposing the program? Amid the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic, more than 60 million Brazilians received an emergency assistance payment that was proposed by Congress against resistance from the executive. This study leverages this unique case to examine if cash transfer programs affect presidential approval under circumstances of unclear responsibility. Survey results showed that while approval ratings increased, the public was divided about who was responsible for the program. Moreover, a survey-experiment that informed respondents about the negotiations between the president and Congress found that information improves views about Congress but does not affect presidential approval. The results suggest that even cash transfer programs may promote limited vertical accountability in contexts of unclear policy responsibility.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the University of Miami

Voters’ ability to punish or reward incumbents based on economic performance can serve as a core mechanism of electoral accountability (Popkin Reference Popkin1991). Yet economic voting may not be a solution for the limitations of vertical accountability because of “contingency dilemmas,” in which economic perceptions and electoral support depend heavily on individual and contextual factors (Anderson Reference Anderson2007). But while economic voting, broadly construed, tends to be conditional, one of its specific empirical manifestations seems to overcome these “contingency dilemmas”: a large body of research shows that large-scale cash transfer programs have important direct effects on voting behavior.

Because cash transfer programs have immediate impact on recipients’ finances and are clearly associated with the government, they provide a neat informational cue that less informed voters can rely on to make electoral choices (Tilley et al. Reference Tilley, Neundorf and Hobolt2018). However, most empirical studies that examine the effects of cash transfer programs on electoral results focus on cases in which the connection between the program and the executive is straightforward. Generally, those cases follow a pattern in which the incumbent party or politician proposes the policy, gathers legislative support, and leads the delivery of the financial benefit. This is especially the case in Latin America, where levels of poverty are high and political systems are president-centered, leading the executive branch to play a central role in setting the agenda, gathering legislative support, and implementing such programs in the different countries across the region (Sugiyama Reference Sugiyama2011). But what happens in contexts where the political process underlying the policy implementation does not follow a clearly executive-centered logic?

A prominent deviant case from the pattern of cash transfer–based accountability is the Auxílio Emergencial paid by the Brazilian federal government during the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic outbreak. During the pandemic, more than 60 million Brazilians received financial aid from the federal government, which was initially proposed by Congress against pushback from the executive. After many rounds of negotiations, the executive conceded and finally cooperated in approving the policy, while President Jair Bolsonaro faced scandals and struggled to gather legislative support within Brazil’s highly fragmented party system. Once the policy was approved by Congress and implemented by the executive, President Bolsonaro’s approval ratings increased, especially among lower-income individuals. Thus the chief executive seems to have enjoyed an increase in approval ratings while initially operating against the implementation of the policy. This disconnect between responsibility and approval may allow political actors to claim credit for policies that they did not propose, or even for policies that they opposed at the legislative level. A potential solution for this problem may be “pointing voters in the right direction” by providing more information on responsibility for the policy. We test this hypothesis in the case of the Auxílio Emergencial and Bolsonaro.

This study uses unique online survey data collected during the pandemic to examine whether the alleged effect of the Auxílio Emergencial on the president’s popularity constitutes an example of failed accountability for cash transfer–based support. We find divided and politicized views about responsibility for the policy, which corroborates the idea that the process of policy implementation followed a distinct dynamic from other cash transfer programs. Moreover, we find that the policy’s effect on presidential approval was substantial among those who saw Bolsonaro as responsible, which accounts for the increase in popularity observed in the polls. Furthermore, using an experimental design, this study shows that informing subjects of the legislature’s initiative in proposing the aid against resistance from the executive does not change views on the president but improves views on Congress. The findings suggest that social assistance as a pocketbook voting mechanism of accountability may also suffer from contingency dilemmas.

Cash Transfer and Political Accountability

Economic voting is the process by which individuals’ economic perceptions affect their views about candidates and parties and inform their vote choice to maximize their prospective economic well-being (Fiorina Reference Fiorina1981; Duch and Stevenson Reference Duch and Stevenson2008). These perceptions of the economy can refer either to voters’ evaluations of their general economic situation (sociotropic) or to their own personal economic situation (egotropic or pocketbook) (Lewis-Beck Reference Lewis-Beck1988).

Theory and evidence suggest that pocketbook economic voting is uncommon. Public opinion studies show that sociotropic evaluations have a stronger association with voting behavior and presidential approval compared to pocketbook evaluations (Fiorina Reference Fiorina1981; Lewis-Beck Reference Lewis-Beck1988; Lewis-Beck and Stegmaier Reference Lewis-Beck and Stegmaier2013). The connection between changes in personal financial conditions and governmental actions involves two distant sets of stimuli in the political environment. The task of connecting those two events requires voters to establish distal attitudinal congruence to vote economically, which makes pocketbook voting a complex task that is best suited for the more sophisticated (Gómez and Wilson Reference Gómez and Matthew Wilson2003, Reference Gómez and Matthew Wilson2006).

However, while sociotropic economic evaluations are more systematically associated with voting behavior, the literature also suggests that the task of connecting general economic outcomes with governmental action is not much simpler than pocketbook voting. Studies show that economic evaluations often have myopic foundations; that is, voters tend to consider limited time frames when assessing the economic performance of governments (Healy and Malhotra Reference Healy and Malhotra2009; Bechtel and Hainmueller Reference Bechtel and Hainmueller2011). Also, voters may often form evaluations based on random events or events that public officials cannot control (Achen and Bartels Reference Achen and Bartels2017; Healy et al. Reference Healy, Malhotra and Hyunjung Mo2010). Moreover, the commonly observed association between sociotropic economic perceptions and political choices can also indicate that voters adjust their views about the economy on the basis of their opinions about the electoral options, and not the contrary (Tilley and Hobolt Reference Tilley and Hobolt2011; Visconti Reference Visconti2019).

Additionally, even if voters form strong and stable opinions about the economy based on reliable economic indicators, one of the key components in economic voting models is the accuracy of the attribution of responsibility for those outcomes. The literature shows that the task of assigning responsibility for economic outcomes to political actors depends heavily on the extent to which the policymaking process is shared by institutions and parties (Powell and Whitten Reference Powell and Whitten1993; Duch and Stevenson Reference Duch and Stevenson2008). Economic voting becomes more common in contexts in which the executive concentrates responsibilities and centralizes power relative to opposition parties, as well as other agencies and levels of government. In the same vein, more recent studies find that factors such as decentralization (León Reference León2011; Guiteras and Mobarak Reference Guiteras and Mushfiq Mobarak2015), globalization (Hellwig and Samuels Reference Hellwig and Samuels2007; Cruz and Schneider 2016), characteristics of the local economy (Campello and Zucco Reference Campello and Zucco2020), and features of presidential systems (Hellwig and Samuels Reference Hellwig and Samuels2008; Samuels Reference Samuels2004; Samuels and Hellwig Reference Samuels and Hellwig2010; Valdini and Lewis-Beck Reference Valdini and Lewis-Beck2018) are key determinants of the extent to which voters attribute responsibility for economic outcomes to the executive.

Surprisingly, a specific type of pocket evaluation seems to overcome many of the limitations associated with economic voting models. An emerging body of scholarship provides evidence that voters tend to reward incumbents who implement large-scale antipoverty cash transfer programs, especially in developing economies. Cash transfer programs (or targeted social assistance) are government policies that seek to help the poor through targeted, means-tested financial aid (Barrientos and Santibáñez Reference Barrientos and Santibáñez2009; Layton and Smith Reference Layton and Erica Smith2015). Since these programs generate immediate and distinguishable changes in recipients’ finances and are usually associated with specific procedures, labels, and locations that signal their connection with government spending, they constitute a more powerful version of pocketbook voting (Tilley et al. Reference Tilley, Neundorf and Hobolt2018). Therefore, cash transfer programs provide a clear and reliable shortcut, in which voters assign political responsibility for the program’s outcome to the executive, especially since cash transfer initiatives usually entail credit claiming.

The evidence in favor of the positive effect of targeted social assistance on presidential approval is overwhelming in Latin America, with examples from countries such as Brazil (Zucco Reference Zucco2008; Licio et al. Reference Licio and Rennó2009; Zucco Reference Zucco and Power2013), Mexico (De La O 2013; but see Imai et al. Reference Imai, King and Velasco Rivera2020), Honduras (Linos Reference Linos2013), Ecuador (Winters Reference Winters2010), and Uruguay (Manacorda et al. Reference Manacorda, Miguel and Vigorito2011).Footnote 1 All in all, while some researchers are inclined to see cash transfer programs as clientelistic practices, or at least as policies that can potentially be –hijacked for electoral purposes, economic voting based on cash transfer programs is generally seen as a type of pocketbook voting that can serve as positive reinforcement for incumbents and, after all, as a mechanism of accountability (Ashworth Reference Ashworth2012; Pavão Reference Pavão2016; Tilley et al. Reference Tilley, Neundorf and Hobolt2018).Footnote 2

Cash transfer programs tend to center on the executive branch throughout the different stages—agenda setting, policy formulation, selection, and implementation (Hoefer Reference Hoefer2021)—of their policy process. In the case of Brazil, different versions of such programs followed an executive-centered logic, with the federal government proposing the program, gathering support in the legislative arena, and carrying out implementation (Sugiyama Reference Sugiyama2011; Zucco Reference Zucco2013). It is particularly important that the federal government was able to design Bolsa Família, Brazil’s largest CCT program to date, while bypassing state-level authorities that would otherwise have undermined the program’s effectiveness (Fenwick Reference Fenwick2009). In this sense, the findings from the literature on the electoral effects of cash transfer programs rescue the connection between economic voting and democratic accountability by looking at those programs as instances where standard barriers to correct assessment and attribution—such as the multitude of actors participating in decisionmaking or the scarcity of information—are mostly absent.

However, little research has been conducted in more complex contexts in which the policy process makes it harder for voters to discern political responsibility for cash transfer programs. More specifically, a puzzling situation emerges when the executive participates in the policy process without exercising proposal power; that is, without being the agent that sets the agenda and initiates the legislative process. In this context, the executive participates in the policy process as an agent with formal veto power or when legally bound with the task of delivering the policy once it becomes law. From the policy process standpoint, an executive that does not initiate the policy process but “goes along” by not exercising full veto power and abiding by the law in implementation has limited causal bearing on the policy.

While proposal power signals commitment to vertical accountability, failing to exercise the veto or carrying on a mandate to implement the policy come because of horizontal accountability, in which the executive cooperates due to its political and legal liability to the other branches of government. Evidence from the scholarship, however, shows that voters tend to concentrate the rewards for collective decisionmaking on political actors with proposal power, rather than on those with veto power (Duch and Stevenson Reference Duch and Stevenson2013; Duch et al. Reference Duch, Przepiorka and Stevenson2015). Moreover, when policy delivery is largely influenced and performed by “non–electorally accountable officials” (Duch and Stevenson Reference Duch and Stevenson2008), such as the federal and local bureaucracies that operate as intermediaries in cash transfer programs, it is rational for voters to discount the reward they would offer to “electorally accountable officials” at the executive level.

All in all, when voters reward the executive for a policy that it did not propose and that it even opposed at the legislative level, they largely misattribute political responsibility. In other words, when the executive cooperates by not using its veto power and by carrying on implementation as mandated by law, it is responding to horizontal accountability pressures rather than to voter preferences. Therefore, it is not clear if cash transfer programs are immune to the conventional shortcomings of economic voting or if they, too, can be subject to “contingency dilemmas” that would make them a less effective mechanism of accountability. By examining a case in which the executive is not responsible for the design and approval of the program, this study sheds light on this question.

The Case of Brazil’s Auxílio Emergencial

The implementation of the Auxílio Emergencial during the COVID-19 pandemic in Brazil is an example of a cash transfer program with complex attribution of responsibility. In addition to Brazil’s large territory and population, the management of a collective response to a disease outbreak poses challenges that are magnified by the country’s high levels of income inequality and poverty (Aquino et al. Reference Aquino and Ismael Henrique Silveira2020). More specifically, the collective measures of social distancing recommended by health experts, such as sheltering in place, closing schools, and reducing economic activity, have disproportionately negative effects on the most vulnerable populations. In this sense, the country’s existing cash transfer program, Bolsa Família, was deemed by specialists as insufficient to provide a safety net for the wider group of the population affected by growing rates of unemployment and diminished economic activity during the pandemic.

Although experts, legislators, and state and local-level authorities concurred about the need to implement large-scale social distancing measures combined with financial relief to citizens since late February 2020, the executive sent ambiguous signals about the course of action. In the first half of March, while Health Minister Luiz Henrique Mandetta seemed aligned with most forces in the political system in favor of following the guidelines from the World Health Organization, President Bolsonaro’s stance became increasingly more explicit against social distancing measures and favorable to the idea that the regular functioning of the economy was preferable to providing citizens with extra government aid. Health Minister Mandetta was discharged less than a month after Bolsonaro gave a nationally televised address on March 24, in which he trivialized the outbreak and spoke openly against social distancing measures. Due to his handling of the pandemic, Bolsonaro was vehemently criticized by the media and the majority of political elites.

At that stage, the executive’s ambivalent response to the pandemic, in combination with constant political turmoil—which peaked with the resignation of popular Minister of Justice Sergio Moro—led to a decrease in Bolsonaro’s approval ratings. These crises reinforced the weakness of Bolsonaro’s fragile legislative coalition within Brazil’s highly fragmented lower house. After weeks of pressure from different sectors of society, and especially from members of both chambers of Congress, the executive proposed a financial assistance payment of 200 reais (about 39 US dollars at the time) to be paid to eligible groups of Brazilian citizens over three months. The proposal produced an immediate negative reaction from Congress, governors, and mayors, who campaigned for a higher monthly payment. Later in March, the bill written by Congress member Eduardo Barbosa and introduced by rapporteur Marcelo Aro proposed a total payment of 500 reais for three months, which received overwhelming support from different sectors of society. Given the imminent defeat, the executive reopened negotiations and, to change perceptions about its resistance to a widely popular measure, proposed a last-minute change to increase the monthly payment to 600 reais.

After both the Chamber of Deputies and the Senate approved the bill, the executive further delayed the implementation of the policy by negotiating funding sources. The first payment was available for withdrawal on April 27, nearly two months after the first confirmed case of COVID-19 in Brazil and four weeks after the bill was approved by Congress. Nearly one month later, the federal government launched a campaign on social media that attributed credits from the program to Bolsonaro and was followed by similar messages from the president and those close to him. Table 1 presents a timeline of these events.

Table 1. Timeline of Events Involving the Creation of the Auxílio Emergencial

The events from the initial negotiation to the later credit claiming battle for the Auxílio Emergencial impose a particularly difficult task in attributing responsibility for government policy. Although the executive has, by design, an active role in shaping the policy during the legislative process and holds primary administrative responsibility for its delivery, it was Congress, operating through nearly unanimous legislative support, that exercised proposal power that realized the Auxílio Emergencial as a policy. While legislators were proactive in pressuring in favor of the program, as well as for a larger amount to be paid, the executive was passive and mostly responsive to horizontal pressures rather than to voter preferences.

This proposition is supported by an exercise of counterfactual reasoning. If Congress had not exercised proposal power in the policy process, the likely outcome would have been either no policy promoted or a cash amount much lower than the one delivered, as the executive first signaled. Alternatively, if the executive had exercised proposal power and made efforts to push it through the legislative process, the result might have been a very similar outcome to the one observed. Therefore, members of Congress were politically responsible for the existence of the program, while the executive was legally bound to carry through its delivery to the public.

Nevertheless, Bolsonaro’s approval ratings increased after the start of the Auxílio Emergencial, particularly among poorer respondents. After a couple of months of payments, the rate of respondents who disapproved of his administration dropped from 44 percent to 34 percent, while his approval rating rose from 32 percent to 37 percent in August 2020 and remained steady in December of the same year, which represented the highest level of approval since Bolsonaro took office.Footnote 3

This variation in approval is striking not simply because of its rapid growth despite the poor handling of the pandemic, but also because of larger shifts within income groups. While wealthier voters became disenchanted with Bolsonaro’s handling of the public health crisis, his approval ratings among the larger share of poorer individuals increased substantially. Specifically, the percentage of poorer respondents (i.e., family income was up to twice the minimum wage) who disapproved of the president dropped from 45 percent before payments were made to 27 percent in the December poll. In contrast, among the middle class (i.e., those who earn between three and five times the minimum wage), the disapproval rate only dropped from 47 to 41 percent in the same period, and increased from 29 percent to 41 percent in the entire period from December 2019 to December 2020. These trends in presidential approval indicate that despite evidence that the federal government mismanaged the several aspects of the pandemic (Ferigato et al. Reference Ferigato, Michelle Fernández and Ilana Ambrogi2020; Smith Reference Smith2020), Bolsonaro enjoyed a rise in popularity due to a policy that his government initially had opposed. Overall, Bolsonaro’s levels of support slightly improved while his base shifted from being composed largely of wealthier voters to a more diverse socioeconomic profile.

If the Auxílio Emergencial program contributed to the bump in presidential approval, we should observe recipients of the benefit to be more approving than nonrecipients, ceteris paribus. But because of the unique dynamics, in which the lack of proposal power by the executive created a scenario of unclear responsibilities, we also expect that the general effect of receiving the benefit should be small, given that individuals may attribute credit for the policy to Congress rather than to the president. That is, the increase in approval associated with the program should be driven by those who believe Bolsonaro was responsible for the Auxílio Emergencial.

Again, attribution of responsibility for this cash transfer program was marked by an unusually complex scenario for this type of policy, which means that the challenges voters usually face when assigning responsibility to other economic outcomesalso apply here. For one thing, extensive research shows that levels of information shape individuals’ attitudes and how they process information (Zaller Reference Zaller1992; Bartels Reference Bartels1996; Krause Reference Krause1997), which means that those who were not aware of the process should have had more difficulty in correctly attributing responsibility. In addition, we know that citizens’ evaluations of policies are often formed to be consistent with previously held beliefs and attachments (e.g., attitudes toward parties) (Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Mendes and Tverdova2004; Evans and Andersen Reference Evans and Andersen2006; James and Van Ryzin Reference James and Van Ryzin2017; Wlezien et al. Reference Wlezien, Franklin and Twiggs1997); this means that attribution of responsibility for the program also was probably influenced by predisposed attitudes toward the president and Congress. Furthermore, politicians may be particularly incentivized to claim credit for a policy even if they are not responsible in scenarios of unclear attribution of responsibility.

Misattribution of responsibility by voters is a problem for democracy to the extent that it provides politicians with incentives to be less concerned with their performance and responsiveness and instead to act more nearly how they please (Achen and Bartels Reference Achen and Bartels2017; Campello and Zucco Reference Campello and Zucco2020). The remedy for this bias seems to be to correct voters’ misattribution. However, existing works are unclear on whether debiasing efforts work. On the one hand, we should expect citizens to update their political views based on new policy information (Fiorina Reference Fiorina1978; Nicholson, Reference Nicholson2011; Bullock Reference Bullock2009; Boudreau and MacKenzie Reference Boudreau and MacKenzie2014), and efforts to debias evaluations can potentially work (Healy et al. Reference Healy, Malhotra and Hyunjung Mo2010). On the other hand, individuals often employ motivated reasoning in political assessments (Bolsen et al. Reference Bolsen, Druckman and Lomax Cook2014; Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006) and often disregard information on incumbent responsibility (Huber et al. Reference Huber, Hill and Lenz2012; Campello and Zucco Reference Campello and Zucco2020).

We contribute to answering this question by testing whether information about the political negotiations for the Auxílio Emergencial influenced views toward the president and Congress, with a survey experiment. We expect that more information about the negotiations in which Congress, rather than the president, had proposal power over the program should harm views on the latter while improving views on the former. Yet the effect of more information should be especially significant among those who did not have strong feelings toward these actors, since they would be less prone to motivated reasoning, and among respondents who were ambivalent or did not know who deserved credit for the program. The experiment was based on an analysis of the relationship between receiving the benefit, attribution of responsibility, and approval.

Observational Analysis

We assessed the impact of the program on views toward the president and Congress with an online survey of one thousand respondents conducted by the polling firm Quaest Consultoria e Pesquisa. Given the infeasibility of conducting face-to-face surveys at the time, the study relied on a sample designed to mirror the online population in terms of income, age, gender, education, and region.Footnote 4 Data collection was carried out between June 14 and 17, 2020, weeks after the federal government made the first two payments of the 600 reais benefit.Footnote 5

The main independent variable of this first analysis is whether respondents received the Auxílio Emergencial, which was measured as a question that briefly described the policy and asked respondents if they had requested and received it. Response options included “request approved and received payments,” “request approved but still waiting for first payment,” “request denied,” and an option for respondents who had not requested the benefit.Footnote 6 The percentage of respondents in the sample with requests approved is 38 percent, the proportion with requests denied is 16 percent, and the proportion who had not requested it is 46 percent.Footnote 7 Benefit recipient status was coded as a binary variable, with those who had the request approved as 1 and all others as 0.

The second key variable in the analysis is who respondents believed to be responsible for the Auxílio Emergencial. The question includes six response options: President Bolsonaro, the National Congress (deputies and senators), governors, and mayors, along with a “don’t know” option. A total of 47.5 percent of respondents indicated that Congress was responsible for the policy, while 38 percent indicated the president. This split in responses reflects the high degree of confusion among citizens about who was politically responsible for the implementation of the policy. Less than 4 percent of respondents indicated that either governors or mayors were responsible for the benefit, while 11 percent did not know. Responsibility for the program was coded in the analyses as a binary variable: those who believed that Bolsonaro or Congress were responsible for the program were coded 1 and other responses 0.

The dependent variables refer to evaluations of the president and Congress. Respondents were asked, “In general, how do you evaluate Jair Bolsonaro’s performance as president of Brazil?,” and “In general, how do you evaluate Congress’s performance?” “Don’t know” responses were coded as middle categories, resulting in a seven-point scale that ranged from “terrible” to “excellent.”

We evaluated the relationship between receiving the benefit and evaluations of the president and Congress by first balancing respondents with respect to pretreatment covariates, using coarsened exact matching (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Iacus, King and Porro2009). The covariates used were family income, sex, age, region, and religious affiliation.Footnote 8 Respondents from higher income brackets (above ten minimum ages) that did not have any recipients were not included in the models.Footnote 9 Moreover, although the survey included a question on the vote choice for president in the 2018 election, we did not attempt to balance the sample based on prior voting, since the vote recall question cannot be considered a pretreatment covariate because it is affected by short-term factors (Van Elsas et al. Reference Van Elsas, Rozemarijn Lubbe, Van Der and Van Der Brug2014; Van Elsas et al. Reference Van Elsas, Miltenburg and van der Meer2016). As the vote choice question asked respondents to recall an event that occurred almost two years before receiving the benefit, we cannot rule out the possibility that those who received the benefit became less likely to declare a past vote for Fernando Haddad, Bolsonaro’s challenger in 2018. Moreover, further examination shows that recipients were not more likely than nonrecipients to consider either Congress or the president as responsible for the benefit.Footnote 10

For estimating the effect of receiving the benefit on presidential and congressional approval, we then ran multivariate OLS regressions on the balanced sample (using the matching weights from the procedure described above), in which we controlled for the matching weights and the same covariates used to balance the sample. Table 2 presents results from six models: three for presidential approval and three for congressional approval. The first modelincludes recipient status only as a predictor, aside from the control variables listed above. In the second, we included the binary indicator of responsibility for the program as a control, and in the third, we interacted the two variables to estimate the extent to which the association between receiving the benefit and approval was conditioned by the attribution of responsibility. For the sake of parsimony, we present only coefficient estimates for the interaction term, and place full model specifications in the appendix (table A2).

Table 2. Impact of Treatment (Recipient) on Presidential and Congressional Approval

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01

Results are coefficients from OLS regressions with matching weights.

Notes: Models do not include respondents from income brackets that do not have program recipients. Dependent variable ranges from 0 to 6 where 6 means “Great.” Coefficient estimates for matching weights, gender, region, income, and religious affiliation are omitted.

The first two models indicate a positive association between receiving the benefit and views on the president, even after controlling for whether the respondent believed that the president was responsible for the program. The third model indicates that this association between the benefit and approval is primarily observed among those who believed that Bolsonaro was responsible for the Auxílio Emergencial. That is, among those who did not believe that Bolsonaro was responsible, there is no significant difference in approval between those who received the benefit and those who did not. In contrast, among those who believed that Bolsonaro was responsible, those who received the benefit expressed significantly higher rates of approval than those who did not. This suggests that the effect of the program would have been substantially larger if a higher share of benefit recipients perceived Bolsonaro as responsible, or inversely, that the effect would have been smaller if more individuals had not seen him as responsible.

Results from models 4 and 5 also indicate that those who received the benefit approved of Congress more than those who did not. However, the association between attribution of responsibility to Congress and approval is not as clear, given that those who believed that Congress was responsible did not approve of Congress more than others. Moreover, there is no significant difference in the association between receiving the benefit and approval of Congress among those who believed that Congress was responsible and those who did not. That is, while those who attributed the Auxílio Emergencial to Congress expressed slightly more positive views toward Congress, the regression coefficients do not give us confidence that there are differences in congressional approval associated with attributing responsibility. Furthermore, the coefficients do not show that the association between the benefit and approval is conditional on credit attribution.Footnote 11

Obviously, it is difficult to infer any causal relationships from this type of analysis. We do not have a time series of respondents to properly evaluate the causal effect of receiving the benefit on later approval of the president and Congress. Yet our results are nonetheless consistent with previous studies on Bolsa Família using similar empirical strategies (Zucco Reference Zucco2013) and with a small panel of telephone respondents that corroborates the positive association between receiving the benefit and more positive views toward the president (Pavão et al. Reference Pavão, Meneses and Borba2020).We also know that the attribution of responsibility may be conditioned by attitudes toward the president in the first place, which may impact our estimates in favor of the conditional relationship. With this limitation in mind, we relied on a survey experiment to examine the role of attributions of responsibility when we showed respondents information about the policymaking process that led to the benefit.

Experimental Analysis

Because voters may face a challenging task in accurately attributing responsibility for the Auxílio Emergencial, our experiment was designed to assess whether providing more information about the process that led to the program affected attitudes toward the president and Congress; that is, how voters distribute the reward for the program between the executive and legislative branches. At the end of the same online survey described previously, we provided a random share of respondents with information about the negotiation process that resulted in the program to potentially debias views on who deserved credit for the program, and then remeasured presidential and congressional approval.

The stimuli designed to clarify responsibility consisted of a short paragraph about the government’s initial proposal of 200 reais, the negotiations led by deputies and senators to increase the value of the benefit, the government’s last move to make it 600, and the president’s delay in implementing the program. The description of events was followed by a question on whether the president’s position of austerity, as opposed to helping the population, was right or wrong. A translation of this description and question is the following:

During the political debates about the Auxílio Emergencial, the Bolsonaro administration proposed a value of 200 reais per month. Deputies and senators from Congress considered it too small and negotiated for the amount to be larger. At last, Bolsonaro proposed an amount of 600 reais, but took a month to sanction the project and vetoed the payment to some groups of workers who were initially going to receive it.

Considering the decisions by the president, with which of the following statements do you agree more?

  • President Bolsonaro was right: one must be careful with the government’s finances during the pandemic.

  • President Bolsonaro was wrong: one must help the population in need during the pandemic.

The stimuli did not explicitly state that Bolsonaro was not politically responsible for the program. The goal of the treatment was mainly to inform subjects about the legislative process of approving the program, one in which the executive took an ambivalent and reactive role and consequently did not exercise proposal power. The first stimulus presents a newspaper-style narrative that conveys Bolsonaro’s hesitance and cooperation while portraying Congress as having an unequivocally proactive role.

This narrative is preferable to less ambiguous approaches, for a few reasons. First, it represents the complex setting and unfolding of the events while conveying factually accurate information; that is, without omitting Bolsonaro’s few cooperative actions, even if taken due to external pressure. Second, since the goal of the treatment is to inform rather than to polarize, a more explicit statement against Bolsonaro could instead alienate his supporters while producing acquiescence among his detractors. Although the polarization effect could potentially boost the treatment, it would not be related to learning. Moreover, while the paragraph about the negotiation process focused on the actions of the president and Congress, the follow-up question on whether Bolsonaro’s position was right was included to engage respondents and clarify that he did not prioritize those in need. It is important to note that for analyzing the effects of the additional information on views toward the president and Congress, we were not interested in the responses to the question, as it was part of the manipulation.

After receiving the stimuli, respondents in the treatment group were again asked in separate questions the extent to which they approved of the president and Congress. Those in the control group were not shown the stimuli, but were asked about approval. As such, we had measures of approval conducted early and late in the questionnaire, with the stimuli shown to a group of respondents just before the second measurement. Our expectations are that informing respondents about the process should clarify responsibility for the program and redistribute views on responsibilities about its implementation. Consequently, we should observe a decrease in the approval level of the president and an increase in the approval level of Congress. These effects should be stronger among respondents who do not have strong predisposed attitudes toward the president and Congress and among respondents who do not know who is responsible for the program.

The randomization of the treatment led to uneven groups and some imbalance. Just over 60 percent of respondents (602 respondents) were assigned to the control group and 40 percent to the treatment. Our balance test based on a regression model showed that the groups were not balanced, on the basis of a few variables.Footnote 12 Due to the imbalances, our analyses were conducted with multivariate regressions that included a set of sociodemographic variables, recipient status, and initial measures of approval as controls. While including covariates as controls in the analysis does not often make the estimates more credible relative to the unadjusted model (Mutz et al. Reference Mutz, Pemantle and Pham2019), the imbalance with respect to the lagged dependent variable suggests that a model without covariate adjustment would lead to biased results.Footnote 13

Our first analysis looks at the general impact of receiving the treatment on presidential and congressional approval. That is, our dependent variables are levels of approval of the president and Congress, measured with the same scale described in the observational analyses. Given that we included the pretreatment measures as predictors in the model, we estimated the change in approval associated with being in the treatment group. We again present a short version of the results in table 3 and place the full regression in the appendix (table A11).

Table 3. Impact of Treatment (Vignette) on Presidential and Congressional Approval

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01

Results are coefficients from OLS regressions.

Dependent variable ranges from 0 to 6 where 6 means “Great.” Coefficient estimates for recipient and sociodemographic controls are omitted.

The first model indicates that receiving the treatment is not associated with a drop in the president’s approval.Footnote 14 In contrast, the second model indicates that being in the treatment group is associated with a significant increase in approval of Congress. More precisely, views on Congress among those in the treatment group improved by roughly 0.38 in the seven-point scale, on average, relative to those in the control group.Footnote 15 This suggests that the treatment designed to inform respondents—that is, description of the negotiating process and question about the position of the president—had a positive effect on views toward Congress. The results from both models also show a high correlation (i.e., consistency) between the first and second measures of approval, especially regarding the president.Footnote 16

The effect of the stimuli on approval of the president and Congress is also likely not to be the same across individuals.Footnote 17 Respondents who already had a positive view of Congress tended to be less affected by the stimuli. That is, when we evaluate the effect of the treatment as conditional on the initial level of approval of Congress, we find that the treatment only affects opinions among those who already disapproved of Congress. In contrast, the effect of the stimuli on approval of the president is consistently null, apart from those who believed that Bolsonaro was doing a terrible job, which is probably explained by flooring effects. Figure1presents the marginal effect of receiving the treatment for each starting level of approval, again controlling for sociodemographic characteristics, approval of the other institution, and whether the respondent received the benefit.

The graph on the left shows that the only statistically significant effect of the treatment was among those who initially thought the president was doing a terrible job. Although significant, the effect is small and potentially suffers from bias, given that respondents were not able express a lower rating because of the limits of the scale (flooring effect). The graph on the right indicates that the treatment had a significant effect on approval of Congress among respondents who picked the three response options associated with disapproval in the pretreatment measure. In other words, the difference in approval of Congress between those in the treatment and control groups is only significant among those who had a strong or slightly negative initial opinion of Congress. One interpretation of this result is that the positive stimulus about Congress is more informative for these groups.

A better test of the informational mechanism is a comparison of the treatment effect between respondents who did not know who was responsible for the Auxílio and respondents who believed that either the president or Congress was responsible. That is, we assessed the effect of the stimuli conditional on who respondents believed was responsible for the Auxilio before the experiment. Figure 2 shows the marginal effect of the stimuli on presidential and congressional approval among those who believed that Congress was responsible, those who thought Bolsonaro was responsible, and those who answered governors, mayors, or “do not know.”

Figure 1. Marginal Effects of Treatment Conditional on Previous Approval of President and Congress

Figure 2. Marginal Effects of Treatment Conditional on Previous View of Who Was Responsible for the Auxílio Emergencial

Once again, the results of heterogeneous effects of the treatment on presidential approval indicate that views on the president are not significantly affected, even among respondents who do not think Congress or Bolsonaro is responsible for the program. Nevertheless, the graph on the right shows that while the effect of the stimuli is positive and significant for all three groups, its effect is larger among those who do not know or attribute to governors and mayors. In other words, the increase in the level of approval of Congress associated with learning about the negotiation process that led to the program is larger among those who were initially ambivalent or least informed about it.

Overall, the results partially corroborate our hypothesis that more information about the process of policy approval and implementation can affect attitudes about political actors through the assignment of responsibilities, since only views on Congress were affected by the experimental stimuli.Footnote 18 The results from heterogeneous effects tests, particularly the second, give support to our hypothesis that those less prone to motivated reasoning are more likely to be affected by the new information, even though this is only the case for views on Congress.

Conclusions

Existing scholarship that investigates the electoral effects of cash transfer programs finds substantive effects in favor of incumbents among recipients. While more general forms of economic perceptions tend to have conditional effects on voting behavior (Anderson Reference Anderson2007), cash transfer programs promote distinguishable changes in voters’ personal finances that are clearly connected to government spending (Tilley et al. Reference Tilley, Neundorf and Hobolt2018). Therefore, the extent to which some voters engage in this type of pocketbook economic voting provides an important mechanism of democratic accountability. However, while the literature focuses primarily on cases in which voters can easily assign credit for the cash transfer programs, fewer studies have explored cases in which responsibility is not as clear.

The case of the Auxílio Emergencial implemented during the COVID-19 pandemic in Brazil provides a unique opportunity to investigate the extent to which cash transfer programs are immune to the “contingency dilemmas” that undermine economic voting as an accountability mechanism. Using survey data, this study found that recipients of the program indeed reward the president when assessing his performance. However, the political initiative for the program in this case was led by Congress against pushback from the executive. Due to this confusion, the survey shows, respondents were initially divided with respect to identifying whether Congress or the president had proposal power over the policy.

Using a survey experiment, this study also showed that informing subjectsabout the negotiation process for the Auxílio Emergencial improved approval of Congress, especially among individuals who did not know who was responsible for the program. On the other hand, the information treatment about the important role Congress played in the creation of the program despite the pushback from the president did not affect presidential approval. This null result seems to be explained in that attitudes toward Bolsonaro tend to be stronger and more stable relative to attitudes toward Congress. All in all, the more complex context behind the implementation of the Auxílio Emergencial led a large portion of voters to misattribute responsibility and misplace rewards for the program. This suggests that even in the case of a cash transfer program, accountability may be limited by a complex informational environment and voters’ previously held beliefs.

These results have important implications for the study of accountability via economic voting and cash transfer programs. The results corroborate scholarly findings on the potential limitations of accountability based on economic voting (Achen and Bartels Reference Achen and Bartels2017; Campello and Zucco Reference Campello and Zucco2020; Novaes and Schiumerini Reference Novaes and Schiumerini2022). More specifically, while the literature on cash transfer–based voting successfully shows that voters connect those programs with incumbent evaluations under circumstances that facilitate that connection (Layton and Smith Reference Layton and Erica Smith2015), our findings suggest that some of the same challenges faced by other types of economic voting can apply to cash transfer programs. The case of the Auxílio Emergencial provides a unique setting where, instead of exercising its traditional leading role in proposing the policy, the executive took a passive stance and decided to “free ride” on the electoral benefits from the program pushed by the legislature. Consequently, we observe a lack of clarity among the public related to attributing responsibility for the program, which made many Brazilians rely on existing political allegiances and place “undeserved credit” for the implementation of the policy.

Also, given the strength of attitudes toward incumbents, and especially toward presidents in Latin America, it is difficult to correct misattributions of responsibility by informing voters about the political processes that generated policy outcomes (Campello and Zucco Reference Campello and Zucco2020). As the study results show, even when voters get the information, they only update their opinions about actors toward which they did not have stable attitudes to begin with. These findings suggest that while partisanship often tends to limit the extent of performance-based voting in more developed democracies, a similar kind of motivated reasoning can take place in contexts with weaker parties but with a personalized structure of political competition.

Finally, these results suggest that in situations that demand large amounts of government spending, complex political environments with divided governments may incentivize incumbents to defect and claim credit for policies for which such credit is not due. Our findings show that presidents can still enjoy an increase in support in such contexts, even if they had minimal impact on shaping the policy that benefits voters.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/lap.2023.17

Conflict of interest

All authors declare no conflict of interest.

Supporting Information

Additional supporting materials may be found with the online version of this article at the publisher’s website: Appendix. For replication data, see the authors’ file on the Harvard Dataverse website: https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataverse/laps

Footnotes

1. In the case of Brazil, while both individual and aggregate evidence points to substantial effects of Bolsa Família, there has been disagreement over this effect (Bohn Reference Bohn2011; Zucco and Power Reference Zucco2013). Some scholars claim that while there is a proincumbent effect among recipients, there is also an anti-incumbent effect among nonrecipients (Corrêa Reference Corrêa2015; Corrêa and Cheibub Reference Corrêa and Antonio Cheibub2016).

2. Additional, extensive research focuses on other nonelectoral political consequences of cash transfer programs, such as their effects on democratic legitimacy and citizenship (Hunter and Sugiyama Reference Hunter and Borges Sugiyama2014; Layton et al. Reference Layton, Donaghy and Rennó2017).

3, Surveys were conducted by Datafolha, one of the largest and best-known polling firms in Brazil (Datafolha 2020a–e).

4. See appendix table A1 for descriptive statistics about the sample. The sample resembles the data from a telephone survey conducted a few weeks earlier by Ipespe (XP Investimentos Reference Investimentos2020), in which most differences in key sociodemographic variables fell within the margin of error of 3 percent.

5. As the polling company collected the data as part of its monthly public opinion tracking and later shared the nonidentifiable data with the authors, both FGV’s and UNCC’s IRB deemed it as not requiring review.

6. See appendix for full instrument in Portuguese.

7. A poll conducted three months later, September 2020, showed similar percentages.

8. Cut points for age (a continuous variable) were used to establish the exact matches. The coarsened exact matching strategy was more successful in achieving balance between treatment and control groups than using propensity scores matching. See appendix tables A3 and A4 for a comparison.

9. Eligibility criteria for the program included both a maximum family income and a maximum per capita income requirement. Due to the latter requirement, there is not necessarily a direct correspondence between the income categories from the survey and eligibility for the program.

10. See appendix tables A6 and A7 for results.

11. Results from OLS models without a balanced sample show similar results (appendix table A5). Also, the results do not show that separating respondents who were denied the benefit were statistically different from those who did not apply for it (appendix table A8). Furthermore, while the effects tend to be stronger for women, the interaction term with sex is not statistically significant (appendix table A9).

12. See appendix table A10 for results.

13. The unadjusted estimates for ATEs are available in appendix table A15.

14. Power analysis indicates that the lack of statistical significance cannot be attributed to the sample size, as the required sample size for the effect (Cohen’s d of 0.02) would be approximately 47 thousand observations. That analysis is available in the replication codes.

15. The difference in results between presidential and congressional approval also cannot be attributed to differet baseline levels of approval, since average approvals before the vignette were 2.14 and 2.32, respectively.

16. We find no clear difference in effects based on gender. See appendix table A17.

17. Given the limited sample size, the subgroup differences observed here must be taken with caution, suggesting rather than testing patterns of relationships.

18. We also conducted an equivalent analysis of heterogeneous effects of the stimuli conditional on previous attribution of responsibility only among those who received the benefit. Overall, the results are similar. See appendix table A17.

References

Achen, Christopher H., and Bartels, Larry M.. 2017. Democracy for Realists: Why Elections Do Not Produce Responsive Governments. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, Christopher J. 2007. The End of Economic Voting? Contingency Dilemmas and the Limits of Democratic Accountability. Annual Review of Political Science 10: 271–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, Christopher J., Mendes, Silvia M., and Tverdova, Yuliya V.. 2004. Endogenous Economic Voting: Evidence from the 1997 British Election. Electoral Studies 23, 4: 683708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aquino, Estela M. L., Ismael Henrique Silveira, Julia Moreira Pescarini, et al. 2020. Social Distancing Measures to Control the COVID-19 Pandemic: Potential Impacts and Challenges in Brazil. Ciencia & Saúde Coletiva 25: 2423–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashworth, Scott. 2012. Electoral Accountability: Recent Theoretical and Empirical Work. Annual Review of Political Science 15: 183201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrientos, Armando, and Santibáñez, Claudio. 2009. New Forms of Social Assistance and the Evolution of Social Protection in Latin America. Journal of Latin American Studies 41, 1: 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartels, Larry M. 1996. Uninformed Votes: Information Effects in Presidential Elections. American Journal of Political Science 40, 1: 194230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, Michael M., and Hainmueller, Jens. 2011. How Lasting Is Voter Gratitude? An Analysis of the Short- and Long-Term Electoral Returns to Beneficial Policy. American Journal of Political Science 55, 4: 852–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, Matthew, Iacus, Stefano, King, Gary, and Porro, Giuseppe. 2009. CEM: Coarsened Exact Matching in Stata. Stata Journal 9, 4: 524–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohn, Simone R. 2011. Social Policy and Vote in Brazil: Bolsa Família and the Shifts in Lula’s Electoral Base. Latin American Research Review 46, 1: 5479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolsen, Toby, Druckman, James N., and Lomax Cook, Fay. 2014. The Influence of Partisan Motivated Reasoning on Public Opinion. Political Behavior 36: 235–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boudreau, Cheryl, and MacKenzie, Scott A.. 2014. Informing the Electorate? How Party Cues and Policy Information Affect Public Opinion About Initiatives. American Journal of Political Science 58, 1: 4862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bullock, John G. 2009. Partisan Bias and the Bayesian Ideal in the Study of Public Opinion. Journal of Politics 71, 3: 1109–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campello, Daniela, and Zucco, Cesar. 2020. The Volatility Curse: Exogenous Shocks And Representation in Resource-Rich Democracies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corrêa, Diego Sanches. 2015. Conditional Cash Transfer Programs, the Economy, and Presidential Elections in Latin America. Latin American Research Review 50, 2: 6385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corrêa, Diego Sanches, and Antonio Cheibub, José. 2016. The Anti–Incumbent Effects of Conditional Cash Transfer Programs. Latin American Politics and Society 58, 1: 4971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruz, Cesi, and Schneider, Christina J.. 2017. Foreign Aid and Undeserved Credit Claiming. American Journal of Political Science 61, 2: 396408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Datafolha. 2020b. Aprovação a Ministério da Saúde cresce enquanto sobe reprovação a Bolsonaro na crise do coronavírus. April 3. https://datafolha.folha.uol.com.br/opiniaopublica/2020/04/1988648-aprovacao-a-ministerio-da-saude-cresce-enquanto-sobe-reprovacao-a-bolsonaro-na-crise-do-coronavirus.shtml Google Scholar
Datafolha. 2020c. Bolsonaro é aprovado por 33 percent, e 45 percent apoiam processo de impeachment. April 28. https://datafolha.folha.uol.com.br/opiniaopublica/2020/04/1988698-bolsonaro-e-aprovado-por-33-e-45-apoiam-processo-de-impeachment.shtml Google Scholar
Datafolha. 2020d. Governadores e Ministério da Saúde superam Bolsonaro na crise do vírus. March 23. https://datafolha.folha.uol.com.br/opiniaopublica/2020/03/1988618-governadores-e-ministerio-da-saude-superam-bolsonaro-na-crise-do-virus.shtml Google Scholar
Datafolha. 2020e. Piora a avaliação do Ministério da Saúde na crise do coron-avírus. May 29. https://datafolha.folha.uol.com.br/opiniaopublica/2020/05/1988734-50-reprovam-desempenho-de-bolsonaro-na-crise-do-coronavirus.shtml Google Scholar
De La, O, Ana, L. 2013. Do Conditional Cash Transfers Affect Electoral Behavior? Evidence from a Randomized Experiment in Mexico. American Journal of Political Science 57, 1: 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duch, Raymond M., and Stevenson, Randolph T.. 2008. The Economic Vote: How Political and Economic Institutions Condition Election Results. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duch, Raymond M., and Stevenson, Randolph T.. 2013. Voter Perceptions of Agenda Power and Attribution of Responsibility for Economic Performance. Electoral Studies 32, 3: 512–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duch, Raymond, Przepiorka, Wojtek, and Stevenson, Randolph. Responsibility Attribution for Collective Decision Makers. 2015. American Journal of Political Science 59, 2: 372–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, Geoffrey, and Andersen, Robert. 2006. The Political Conditioning of Economic Perceptions. Journal of Politics 68, 1: 194207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenwick, Tracy Beck. 2009. Avoiding Governors: The Success of Bolsa Família. Latin American Research Review 44, 1: 102–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferigato, Sabrina, Michelle Fernández, Melania Amorim, Ilana Ambrogi, L et al. 2020. The Brazilian Government’s Mistakes in Responding to the COVID-19 Pandemic. Lancet 396, 10263: 1636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiorina, Morris P. 1978. Economic Retrospective Voting in American National Elections: A Micro-analysis. American Journal of Political Science 22, 2: 426–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiorina, Morris P. 1981. Retrospective Voting in American National Elections. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Gómez, Brad T., and Matthew Wilson, J.. 2003. Causal Attribution and Economic Voting in American Congressional Elections. Political Research Quarterly 56, 3: 271–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, Brad T., and Matthew Wilson, J.. 2006. Cognitive Heterogeneity and Economic Voting: A Comparative Analysis of Four Democratic Electorates. American Journal of Political Science 50, 1: 127–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guiteras, Raymond P., and Mushfiq Mobarak, Ahmed. 2015. Does Development Aid Undermine Political Accountability? Leader and Constituent Responses to a Large-Scale Intervention. Working paper 21434. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, Andrew, and Malhotra, Neil. 2009. Myopic Voters and Natural Disaster Policy. American Political Science Review 103, 3: 387406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, Andrew J., Malhotra, Neil, and Hyunjung Mo, Cecilia. 2010. Irrelevant Events Affect Voters’ Evaluations of Government Performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107, 29: 12804–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hellwig, Timothy, and Samuels, David. 2007. Voting in Open Economies: The Electoral Consequences of Globalization. Comparative Political Studies 40, 3: 283306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hellwig, Timothy, and Samuels, David. Electoral Accountability and the Variety of Democratic Regimes. 2008. British Journal of Political Science 38, 1: 6590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoefer, Richard. 2021. The Surprising Usefulness of the Policy Stages Framework. Journal of Policy Practice and Research 2: 141–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, Gregory A., Hill, Seth J., and Lenz, Gabriel S.. 2012. Sources of Bias in Retrospective Decision Making: Experimental Evidence on Voters’ Limitations in Controlling Incumbents. American Political Science Review 106, 4: 720–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunter, Wendy, and Borges Sugiyama, Natasha. 2014. Transforming Subjects into Citizens: Insights from Brazil’s Bolsa Família. Perspectives on Politics 12, 4: 829–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imai, Kosuke, King, Gary, and Velasco Rivera, Carlos. 2020. Do Nonpartisan Programmatic Policies Have Partisan Electoral Effects? Evidence from Two Large-scale Experiments. Journal of Politics 82, 2: 714–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, Oliver, and Van Ryzin, Gregg G.. 2017. Motivated Reasoning About Public Performance: An Experimental Study of How Citizens Judge yhe Affordable Care Act. Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 27, 1: 197209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, George A. 1997. Voters, Information Heterogeneity, and the Dynamics of Aggregate Economic Expectations. American Journal of Political Science 41, 4: 11701200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layton, Matthew L., and Erica Smith, Amy. 2015. Incorporating Marginal Citizens and Voters: The Conditional Electoral Effects of Targeted Social Assistance in Latin America. Comparative Political Studies 48, 7: 854–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layton, Matthew L., Donaghy, Maureen M., and Rennó, Lúcio R.. 2017. Does Welfare Provision Promote Democratic State Legitimacy? Evidence from Brazil’s Bolsa Família Program. Latin American Politics and Society 59, 4: 99120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
León, Sandra. 2011. Who Is Responsible for What? Clarity of Responsibilities in Multilevel States: The Case of Spain. European Journal of Political Research 50, 1: 80109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis-Beck, Michael S. 1988. Economics and the American Voter: Past, Present, Future. Political Behavior 10: 521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis-Beck, Michael S., and Stegmaier, Mary. 2013. The VP-Function Revisited: A Survey of the Literature on Vote and Popularity Functions After Over 40 Years. Public Choice 157: 367–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Licio, Elaine Cristina, Rennó, Lucio R., and Henrique Carlos de O. de Castro. 2009. Bolsa Família e voto na eleição presidencial de 2006: em busca do elo perdido. Opinião Pública 15: 3154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linos, Elizabeth. 2013. Do Conditional Cash Transfer Programs Shift Votes? Evidence from the Honduran PRAF. Electoral Studies 32, 4: 864–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manacorda, Marco, Miguel, Edward, and Vigorito, Andrea. 2011. Government Transfers and Political Support. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 3, 3: 128.Google Scholar
Mutz, Diana C., Pemantle, Robin, and Pham, Philip. 2019. The Perils of Balance Testing in Experimental Design: Messy Analyses of Clean Data. American Statistician 73, 1: 3242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholson, Stephen P. 2011. Dominating Cues and the Limits of Elite Influence. Journal of Politics 73, 4: 1165–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novaes, Lucas M., and Schiumerini, Luis. 2022. Commodity Shocks and Incumbency Effects. British Journal of Political Science 52, 4: 16891708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavão, Nara. 2016. Conditional Cash Transfer Programs and Electoral Accountability: Evidence from Latin America. Latin American Politics and Society 58, 2: 7499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavão, Nara, Meneses, Mariana, and Borba, Lucas. 2020. Crise do Coronavírus e atitudes políticas no Brasil: um estudo longitudinal. Paper presented at the Fundação Getulio Vargas Political Science Seminar, December 3, 2020.Google Scholar
Popkin, Samuel L. 1991. The Reasoning Voter: Communication and Persuasion in Presidential Campaigns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powell, G. Bingham Jr., and Whitten, Guy D.. 1993. A Cross–national Analysis of Economic Voting: Taking Account of the Political Context. American Journal of Political Science 37, 2: 391414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samuels, David. 2004. Presidentialism and Accountability for the Economy in Comparative Perspective. American Political Science Review 98, 3: 425–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samuels, David, and Hellwig, Timothy. 2010. Elections and Accountability for the Economy: A Conceptual and Empirical Reassessment. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 20, 4: 393419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Amy Erica. 2020. COVID vs. Democracy: Brazil’s Populist Playbook. Journal of Democracy 31, 4: 7690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, Natasha Borges. 2011. The Diffusion of Conditional Cash Transfer Programs in the Americas. Global Social Policy 11, 2–3: 250–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taber, Charles S., and Lodge, Milton. 2006. Motivated skepticism in the Evaluation of Political Beliefs. American Journal of Political Science 50, 3: 755–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilley, James, and Hobolt, Sara B.. 2011. Is the Government to Blame? An Experimental Test of How Partisanship Shapes Perceptions of Performance and Responsibility. Journal of Politics 73, 2: 316–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilley, James, Neundorf, Anja, and Hobolt, Sara B.. 2018. When the Pound in People’s Pocket Matters: How Changes to Personal Financial Circumstances Affect Party Choice. Journal of Politics 80, 2: 555–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valdini, Melody E., and Lewis-Beck, Michael S.. 2018. Economic Voting in Latin America: Rules and Responsibility. American Journal of Political Science 62, 2: 410–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Elsas, Erika J., Rozemarijn Lubbe, Tom W. G. Van Der, Meer, and Van Der Brug, Wouter. 2014. Vote Recall: A Panel Study on the Mechanisms that Explain Vote Recall Inconsistency. International Journal of Public Opinion Research 26, 1: 1840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Elsas, Erika J., Miltenburg, Emily M., and van der Meer, Tom W. G.. 2016. If I Recall Correctly: An Event History Analysis of Forgetting and Recollecting Past Voting Behavior. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 26, 3: 253–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visconti, Giancarlo. 2019. Economic Perceptions and Electoral Choices: A Design-Based Approach. Political Science Research and Methods 7, 4: 795813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winters, Matthew. 2010. Exploring Changes over Time in the Targeting of a Cash Transfer Program in Ecuador. Unpublished mss. Department of Political Science, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
Wlezien, Christopher, Franklin, Mark, and Twiggs, Daniel. 1997. Economic Perceptions and Vote Choice: Disentangling the Endogeneity. Political Behavior 19, 1: 717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Investimentos, XP. 2020. Pesquisa XP/Ipespe. Online. June 9 and 11. https://conteudos.xpi.com.br/wp-content/uploads/2020/06/Pesquisa-XP_-2020_06-v2.pdf Google Scholar
Zaller, John R. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zucco, Cesar. 2008. The President’s “New”Constituency: Lula and the Pragmatic Vote in Brazil’s 2006 Presidential Elections. Journal of Latin American Studies 40, 1: 2949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zucco, Cesar. 2013. When Payouts Pay Off: Conditional Cash Transfers and Voting Behavior in Brazil, 2002–10. American Journal of Political Science 57, 4: 810–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zucco, Cesar, and Power, Timothy J.. 2013. Bolsa Família and the Shift in Lula’s Electoral Base, 2002–2006: A Reply to Bohn. Latin American Research Review 48, 2: 324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Timeline of Events Involving the Creation of the Auxílio Emergencial

Figure 1

Table 2. Impact of Treatment (Recipient) on Presidential and Congressional Approval

Figure 2

Table 3. Impact of Treatment (Vignette) on Presidential and Congressional Approval

Figure 3

Figure 1. Marginal Effects of Treatment Conditional on Previous Approval of President and Congress

Figure 4

Figure 2. Marginal Effects of Treatment Conditional on Previous View of Who Was Responsible for the Auxílio Emergencial

Supplementary material: PDF

Batista Pereira et al. supplementary material

Online Appendix

Download Batista Pereira et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 221.3 KB